Verified by Psychology Today

"I Feel Your Pain" (Literally)

New research indicates that physical pain may be socially transferable.

Source: "Perez the Mouse" by Luis Coloma / Flickr / Public Domain

In 1992, presidential candidate Bill Clinton famously told an activist who was heckling him at a rally, “I feel your pain,” and that statement quickly entered the popular vernacular as a clichéd expression of sympathy.

Most of us have at one time or another found ourselves telling someone who is complaining to us about something that we, like Bill Clinton, “feel” his or her pain. Whether our offer of sympathy is intended to be taken sincerely or ironically, however, we almost certainly do not intend for it to be taken literally. No matter how empathetic we may like to imagine ourselves, we cannot actually feel someone else’s physical pain. Or can we?

A recent study at Oregon Health and Science University suggests that physical pain may, indeed, be transferrable from one individual to another. In the Oregon study, one group of mice was subjected to a variety of pain-inducing stimuli (inflammation from a local injection of a noxious substance, and withdrawal from opiates and alcohol). Other “bystander” mice, not subject to the pain-inducing stimuli, were housed in different degrees of proximity to the primary mice in order to test whether or not their pain would be transferred to the bystander mice. All the mice were housed in open cages, but while one group of bystander mice was housed in the same room as the mice suffering inflammation and withdrawal, another group was housed in a separate room.

As expected, the mice that had been subjected to noxious stimuli soon developed hyperalgesia, or a measurable increase in sensitivity to pain. Interestingly, the bystander mice housed in the same room with the hyperalgesic mice exhibited a similar heightened sensitivity to pain. Even without any direct exposure to noxious stimuli, the bystander mice “felt” the pain of the primary mice who were suffering the effects of inflammation and withdrawal.

Not surprisingly, the mice housed in a separate room—cut off from visual and auditory cues related to the pain sensitivity of the other mice—showed no initial signs of hyperalgesia. When a small amount of bedding from the cages of the hyperalgesic mice was placed in the cages of the mice housed in the separate room, however, those mice too began to exhibit hyperalgesia. In the absence of visual and auditory information from the other two groups of mice, olfactory cues from their bedding induced hyperalgesia in the physically separated group of mice. While only one group of mice was actually subjected to a noxious stimulus, all three groups ended up displaying pronounced hyperalgesia. In other words, the pain of the mice in the first group was effectively transferred to the mice in the other two groups.

This study reinforces the conclusions of other studies on the transferability of pain. It covers new ground, however, in its demonstration of olfaction as a major mechanism in the social transfer of pain. There is still a great deal to be learned about the manner in which these cues trigger the experience of hyperalgesia, but a mounting body of evidence suggests that we may, indeed, be able to feel each other’s pain.

So the next time you say to a suffering friend of yours that you feel his or her pain, you can do so with the confidence that, however sincere or otherwise your extension of sympathy might actually be, you are at least expressing a physiological possibility. Of course, given the findings of this most recent study it might be more technically accurate to say that you smell someone’s pain, but however you express it, the ability to experience someone else’s pain is a literal fact rather than a figurative expression.

More from Hal McDonald Ph.D.
More from Psychology Today
Most Popular