Skip to main content
Child Development

Are There Upsides to a Difficult Infant Temperament?

Could "difficult" infant traits just be early manifestations of later strengths?

Key points

  • Existing research on temperament focuses on "difficult" traits like fussiness, intensity, and sleep problems.
  • There may be more to it than just "being difficult," but research has not really looked for positive traits.
  • Parents routinely acknowledge that positive traits (humor, empathy, etc.) coexist with challenging ones.

“Difficult,” “irritable,” “unsettled,” “fussy,” “a handful.” We know these babies (many of us had these babies). They cry louder and longer than others. They have feeding and/or sleeping problems (Owens-Stively et al., 1997; Spruyt et al., 2008). They’re more reactive and less able to calm down without a lot of help (Calkins et al., 2002). According to research, these babies would be considered temperamentally “difficult.”

Research on a “difficult” temperament

Approximately 10 percent of infants can be classified as “difficult” (Thomas & Chess, 1977). Research suggests that a difficult temperament is defined by problems with self-regulation, low sensory thresholds, heightened reactivity, and emotional intensity (Rothbart, 2011). While some positive temperament traits (e.g., sociability, Buss & Plomin, 1984; pleasure/joy, interest/persistence, Goldsmith & Campos, 1982; eagerness/extraversion, Rothbart, 2011) have been included in definitions of temperament, the most used terms overwhelmingly emphasize the more negative aspects of behavior.

Having an infant with a challenging temperament directly impacts parents’ self-confidence (Gordon, 2019; Porter & Hsu, 2003), upending their expectations of parenting and increasing their stress levels (e.g., Oddi et al., 2013). Having a difficult baby increases the risk for depression and anxiety (e.g., Britton, 2011). In the long term, a difficult temperament has been tied to behavioral and emotional problems (Abulizi et al., 2017), obesity (Anzman-Frasca et al., 2012), internalizing/externalizing problems (Valla et al., 2020), and more disrupted sleep (Morales-Muñoz et al., 2020). Truthfully, it sounds bleak.

As a parent of two (now grown) intense, sensitive infants, I know that it can be bleak—but that’s not all it is. Reactive infants can also be deeply feeling, inquisitive, curious, squeaky wheels who demand a higher level of investment from their parents. Is it possible that there are strengths in this package that researchers just haven’t looked for?

Parenting books: spirited, active-alert, highly sensitive, livewire

More balanced descriptions of a “difficult” temperament exist in the world of parenting advice books. A spirited child (Kurcinka, 2015; 2020) is more active, perceptive, persistent, irregular, and intense than mellower children. Active/alert children (Budd, 2003) can be highly active, intense, and controlling, as well as empathic and bright. Highly sensitive children (Aron, 2002) are cautious and easily overwhelmed, but they’re also compassionate, creative, and conscientious. I coined the term livewire to denote a child who seems to have more energetic “current” running through their system (Gordon & West, 2024). They can be alert/engaged, intense, emotionally and physically sensitive, and perceptive.

Though the research on early intelligence has fallen off in recent decades, it identified traits like newborn alertness, generally lower sleep needs, early and high levels of motor activity, persistence, and low tolerance for frustration (Rogers & Silverman, 1998; Silverman, 2001). A qualitative study with parents of school-age identified gifted students asked what these children were like as infants (Rastogi & Gordon, 2019). Not surprisingly, all of them reported that their child had extreme difficulties with sleep and emotional regulation as infants. They also reported that their child had early and intense levels of alertness and motor activity.

I conducted a large parent survey (856 parents of children aged 6 months to 6 years) to assess whether there were positive traits that could be identified in children classified as “difficult” (Gordon, 2019). The results showed that a “difficult” temperament predicted higher levels of perceptiveness and emotional sensitivity (the ability to detect others’ emotions; p<.001). In the older children, it also predicted higher levels of empathy, perfectionism, and valuing justice and fairness (all p<.001).

What parents say

In the same survey, I asked parents to describe their child’s temperament.

  • He is the most fun kid I’ve met at his age—also the most intense and exhausting. He is super smart, intuitive, and loving, but struggles with anger....” (27 months)
  • I'm both amazed and exhausted by how much my child, knows, feels, and does. I question all the time if I'm good enough to handle her.…She's the biggest challenge I've ever faced, but the thing I am most proud of that I have ever done.” (36 months)
  • She’s my Sour Patch Kid—super sassy and sour but can be the sweetest and most polite little girl…It’s like her personality is too big for her little body.” (45 months)

These are some descriptions from parents on a Facebook page for intense, sensitive children:

  • “Fearless, determined, strong, expressive, outgoing, kind to everyone who gives her a chance.”
  • “Passionate, loving, hilarious, and intelligent. Sure, his negative feelings are huge, but all the positive feelings are too! So big happiness, big laughter, everything.”
  • “Boss baby, future CEO, persistent, doesn't-take-no-for-an-answer, verbally gifted, socially smart, a 30-year-old in a 9-year-old body.”
  • “Chatty, strong-willed, headstrong, knows what she wants.”
  • “Firecracker, energetic, chatty/communicative, sensitive, intense.”

Parents know that there’s more to their child than just the challenging traits.

Perceptions of temperament matter

The words used to describe temperament matter because they impact parental perceptions. Having a fussy, colicky, sleep-avoidant newborn is hard enough. Negative perceptions and fears that something is “wrong” (or will be wrong) can set them on a negative trajectory that impacts their parenting choices and experiences. Parents who perceive their infant negatively tend to see more negative behavior (Pauli-Pott et al., 2003).

The lack of helpful information on early temperament also leads parents to worry that their child’s behavior is their fault, that they have “caused” the fussiness or sleep problems. In my survey, I found that in addition to feeling more physically and emotionally exhausted, parents of the “difficult” children rated their own parenting competence significantly lower than those with more easygoing children. They were working harder and, at the same time, thinking that they weren’t doing a good job. Is it possible that a more balanced view of temperament could give parents a more hopeful lens? Could knowledge like this help them feel better about the job they’re doing?

Positive parenting matters

Positive parenting with sensitive children can have an enormous impact. Researchers Pluess and Belsky (2010; 2013) suggest that children with a sensitive, intense temperament respond more strongly to both negative and positive parenting environments—meaning they fare significantly worse in negative contexts but do even better than easygoing children in positive ones. The big research term for this is “differential susceptibility to context.” The same context can have different impacts on a child based on their temperament.

Parents deserve a more balanced view of temperament that highlights the possible strengths that exist beside or underneath the harder ones. Could a more positive light alter some of the negative long-term consequences of a “difficult” temperament? It’s definitely worth finding out.

References

Abulizi, X., Pryor, L., Michel, G., Melchior, M., van der Waerden, J., & EDEN Mother–Child Cohort Study Group. (2017). Temperament in infancy and behavioral and emotional problems at age 5.5: The EDEN mother-child cohort. Plos One, 12(2), e0171971. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171971

Anzman-Frasca, S., Stifter, C. A., & Birch, L. L. (2012). Temperament and childhood obesity risk: A review of the literature. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(9), 732–745. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e31826a119f

Aron, E. (2002). The Highly Sensitive Child: Helping Our Children Thrive When the World Overwhelms Them. Harmony.

Britton, J. R. (2011). Infant temperament and maternal anxiety and depressed mood in the early postpartum period. Women & Health, 51(1), 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/03630242.2011.540741

Budd, L. (2003). Living With the Active-Alert Child. Parenting Press.

Buss, A. & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits. Erlbaum.

Calkins, S. D., Dedmon, S. E., Gill, K. L., Lomax, L. E., & Johnson, L. M. (2002). Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy, 3(2), 175–197. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0302_4

Goldsmith, H. H. & Campos, J. (1982). Toward a theory of infant temperament. In R. Emde & R. Harmon (Eds.), Attachment and Affiliative Systems (pp. 161–193). Plenum Press.

Gordon, M. D. (2019, March). Are there strengths that underlie the challenges of a difficult temperament? Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Conference, Baltimore, Maryland. March 22–24, 2019.

Gordon, M., & West, K. (2024). Why Won’t You Sleep? A Game-Changing Approach for Exhausted Parents of Nonstop, Super-Alert, Big-Feeling Kids. Benbella.

Kurcinka, M. S. (2015). Raising Your Spirited Child (3rd Edition): A Guide for Parents Whose Child Is More Intense, Sensitive, Perceptive, Persistent, and Energetic. William Morrow.

Kurcinka, M. S. (2020). Raising Your Spirited Baby: A Breakthrough Guide to Thriving When Your Baby Is More . . . Alert and Intense and Struggles to Sleep. William Morrow.

Morales-Muñoz, I., Nolvi, S., Virta, M., Karlsson, H., Paavonen, E. J., & Karlsson, L. (2020). The longitudinal associations between temperament and sleep during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 61, 101485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101485

Oddi, K. B., Murdock, K. W., Vadnais, S., Bridgett, D. J., & Gartstein, M. A. (2013). Maternal and infant temperament characteristics as contributors to parenting stress in the first year postpartum. Infant and Child Development, 22(6), 553–579. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1813

Pauli-Pott, U., Mertesacker, B., Bade, U., Haverkock, A., & Beckmann, D. (2003). Parental perceptions and infant temperament development. Infant Behavior and Development, 26(1), 27–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00167-4

Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Children’s differential susceptibility to effects of parenting. Family Science, 1(1), 14–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/19424620903388554

Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2013). Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 901–916. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030196

Porter, C. L., & Hsu, H.-C. (2003). First-time mothers’ perceptions of efficacy during the transition to motherhood: Links to infant temperament. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(1), 54–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.1.54

Rastogi, A., & Gordon, M. D. (2019, March). Difficult or gifted? Qualitative investigation of parents’ experiences of their gifted children as infants. Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Conference, Baltimore, Maryland. March 22–24, 2019.

Rogers, M. T., & Silverman, L. K. (1998). Recognizing Giftedness in Young Children. Gifted Development Center.

Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming Who We Are: Temperament and Personality in Development. Guilford Press.

Silverman, L. (2001). Early Signs of Giftedness. Gifted Development Center.

Spruyt, K., Aitken, R. J., So, K., Charlton, M., Adamson, T. M., & Horne, R. S. C. (2008). Relationship between sleep/wake patterns, temperament, and overall development in term infants over the first year of life. Early Human Development, 84(5), 289–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.07.002

Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and Development. Brunner/Mazel.

Valla, L., Småstuen, M. C., Andenæs, R., Misvær, N., Olbjørn, C., & Helseth, S. (2020). Association between colic and sleep problems in infancy and subsequent development, emotional and behavioral problems: A longitudinal study. BMC Pediatrics, 21(1), 23. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-020-02483-1

advertisement
More from Macall Gordon M.A.
More from Psychology Today