Child Development
The Mother of Communication
How listening to mom from the womb gives us a head start in learning to talk.
Posted May 11, 2025 Reviewed by Jessica Schrader
Key points
- Even in the womb, babies can hear and recognize the rhythm and pitch of their mother’s voice.
- Prenatal exposure to speech helps babies begin learning the patterns of language.
- Babies in the womb have been observed mimicking vowel sounds, suggesting early preparation for speaking.
Mother’s Day brings to mind the importance of moms in our lives, but most of us have no idea that their influence on one of the most important things we’ll ever do—speaking—begins well before we are born.
Listening in the Womb
Though some rudimentary aspects of hearing begin earlier, research suggests that the auditory system—comprising the outer and inner ear as well as the auditory nerve—becomes fully developed by around 26 to 28 weeks of gestation.
So, what exactly do babies hear in their mother's belly? Inside the body, they hear mom’s heartbeat, breathing, and digestive noises. From the outside world, the most prominent and consistent sound they’re exposed to is their mother’s voice, along with occasional music or other environmental sounds.
Because of the insulating properties of the womb and amniotic fluid, the range of sound babies can hear is limited to mainly lower frequencies. This means that elements of speech like pitch and rhythmic patterns are what prenatal babies are most able to detect.
This early exposure to a mother’s voice helps begin a special communicative bond between mother and child. In fact, studies have shown that when babies in utero hear their mother’s voice—played through a loudspeaker placed on the mother’s belly—their heart rate increases, a sign of recognition and engagement. When they hear an unfamiliar female voice, their heart rate decreases.
An Outsized Influence
What’s fascinating is that this early exposure to a mother’s speech appears to prime babies for language learning—a process that begins even before birth.
The limited frequency range of the womb may actually serve a protective and preparatory purpose. It allows babies to tune in to the rhythm, tone, and prosody of speech without being overstimulated by the full complexity of language sounds. This limited speech exposure seems to help babies begin distinguishing between native and non-native speech patterns, and may even aid in recognizing emotional cues in voices.
This early experience gives newborns a kind of “starter kit” for language. Once born, they’re better prepared to take on more complex tasks like identifying vowels, consonants, and other unique characteristics of their native language.
It’s somewhat similar to how vision develops: newborns start out seeing the world in a blurry way, giving their visual systems time to adjust. With speech, though, the training begins in the womb, setting the stage for rapid learning after birth.
Research shows that, soon after birth, babies already distinguish and show a preference for their mother’s voice over a stranger’s. They can also tell the difference between a familiar language and a foreign one, even when both are spoken by someone they’ve never heard before.
Preparing to Speak
Not only does prenatal exposure to speech help babies recognize language patterns, but it may also begin to build the physical foundations for speaking.
Ultrasound studies have observed that fetuses move their mouths in response to vowel sounds, appearing to mimic the mouth movements needed to produce those sounds. This suggests that early exposure to speech may help establish sensorimotor pathways—the neural circuits linking hearing and movement—that will later support speech production.
Forging a Linguistic Bond
Hearing a mother’s voice also appears to play a crucial role in a child’s cognitive, emotional, and social development.
For example, research has shown that premature babies in neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) benefit from hearing their mother’s voice—even if they aren’t able to be held. This auditory exposure can help them feed more effectively, gain weight, and become more socially engaged.
One study found that premature infants who hear their mother’s voice regularly show greater conversational behaviors and go on to have higher language development scores at both 7 and 18 months of age compared to those without that exposure.
Full-term infants, too, show a clear preference for their mother’s voice, which suggests that beyond helping with language development, her voice also soothes and strengthens the bond between mother and baby.
For instance, hearing a mother’s voice has been linked to increased oxytocin levels, the hormone associated with social bonding, especially when the voice includes emotional or supportive cues. One study found that girls undergoing a stressful experience had lower cortisol (stress hormone) levels and higher oxytocin after talking to their mom—but not after texting. The sound of her voice made the difference.
A Lasting Connection
As it turns out, a mother’s voice is not just comforting—it’s foundational. It’s the first sound we come to recognize, the first rhythm we grow familiar with, and the first step in our journey to understanding and using language.
This Mother’s Day, as we reflect on everything moms do for us, it’s worth remembering that their role as our first teacher starts long before we enter the world. Even in the womb, their voices help shape who we are—cognitively, emotionally, and linguistically.
References
Baron-Cohen L, Fearon P, Meins E, et al. Maternal mind-mindedness and infant oxytocin are interrelated and negatively associated with postnatal depression. Development and Psychopathology. Published online 2024:1-12. doi:10.1017/S0954579424001585
Caskey M, Stephens B, Tucker R, Vohr B. Adult Talk in the NICU With Preterm Infants and Developmental Outcomes. (2014). Pediatrics 133 (3): e578–e584.
Chorna, Olena D., et al. (2014). “A pacifier-activated music player with mother’s voice improves oral feeding in preterm infants.” Pediatrics 133.3: 462-468.
DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP (1980) Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science 208(4448):1174–1176.
Hepper, P. G., Scott, D., & Shahidullah, S. (1993). Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice. Developmental Psychobiology, 26(7), 437–450.
Kisilevsky BS, Hains SM, Lee K, Xie X, Huang H, Ye HH, Zhang K, Wang Z. (2003). Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition. Psychol Sci. 14(3):220-4.
Moon, C., Lagercrantz, H., & Kuhl, P. K. (2013). Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: A two-country study. Acta Paediatrica, 102(2), 156–160.
Reissland N, Francis B, Buttanshaw L, Austen JM, Reid V. (2016). Do fetuses move their lips to the sound that they hear? An observational feasibility study on auditory stimulation in the womb. Pilot Feasibility Study. 1;2:14
Seltzer LJ, Prososki AR, Ziegler TE, Pollak SD. (2012). Instant messages vs. speech: hormones and why we still need to hear each other. Evol Hum Behav. 33(1):42-45.