Intelligence
Keeping Pigeons Close to Our Hearts
New research finds that pigeons can classify diseased and normal human hearts.
Posted July 14, 2020
According to the CDC, you are more likely to die from heart disease than from cancer or respiratory diseases. Care providers and researchers work around the clock to fight this gloomy statistic, developing preventative pharmacological and last-resort invasive therapies. However, the effective deployment of these therapies requires a timely and reliable method for detecting the onset of cardiac disease.
If your physician suspects you might be suffering from heart disease, then they will likely recommend that you get a Myocardial Perfusion SPECT test (or MPS for short). Although this test is highly accurate in determining how well blood is flowing through the heart muscle (the myocardium), the decision of whether you will need treatment ultimately rests on how accurately this test is interpreted by your physician. The bad news is that studies have shown that humans’ interpretation of MPS tests is fairly subjective: individual observers are only moderately accurate (they are correct about 85% of the time), and inter-observer agreement is imperfect (different observers agree only 87% of the time).
To avoid this interpretability problem, some researchers have proposed to aid humans’ interpretations using artificial intelligence. In a recent project, we took this proposal one step further, by requesting medical consultation from a feathery friend: the pigeon.
In case you missed it, we have previously reported that pigeons can proficiently classify mammograms and histology samples of human breast tissue as benign or malignant. This time around, using elementary operant conditioning techniques, we trained pigeons to distinguish between MPS tests from normal of abnormal hearts, which had nominal and low perfusion levels, respectively.
Pigeons were initially trained to peck one of two different black-and-white patterns on a touchscreen to report whether the MPS data they were seeing on a computer monitor corresponded with normal or abnormal hearts.
In each training session, pigeons were shown individual MPS images across several trials. On each trial, pigeons had to peck the image multiple times. Having ensured pigeons had enough time to study the image, we presented them with two report buttons located on each side of the image. A final peck to either of the report buttons constituted the pigeon’s choice. If the image shown to them was normal and they pecked the “normal” report button, or if the image shown to them was abnormal and they pecked the “abnormal” button, then we rewarded them with food. However, if the pigeons pecked the incorrect button, then no food was given.
Pigeons succeeded in quickly learning these images, attaining 80% to 85% accuracy. We then set out to discover whether our pigeons could generalize their diagnoses to images they had never seen before. The birds passed this generalization test with flying colors—categorization of novel images was as just accurate as was categorization of familiar images—confirming they had not merely memorized the appropriate response for each image, but rather had acquired true normal/abnormal categories. In a final test, we showed the pigeons black-and-white versions of the same MPS images they had seen in training. Despite these images conveying the identical information as their colored counterparts, pigeons were unable to categorize these images accurately. Clearly, color had played a pivotal part during learning.
In a second experiment, a different cohort of pigeons was trained with black-and-white MPS images from the get-go. Although the absence of color information put a dent in the speed with which our pigeons learned to categorize the images, the birds were again able to transfer their performance to novel images with no loss in accuracy.
It is worth noting that MPS data are not initially colorized; technicians choose whether to colorize these images, and if so, which color map to use. That decision is not trivial. By keeping pigeons close to our hearts and seeing the world through their eyes, we learned a thing or two about the psychological processes underlying humans’ classification of medical images. By using pigeons as surrogate cardiologists, our pair of experiments proved that the presence of color information makes difficult medical classification tasks such as these easier to learn, but it does not necessarily improve the ability to classify new cases.
Victor Navarro and Edward Wasserman
The University of Iowa