Emotional Contagion
Therapy Dogs and Joy Contagion in the Classroom
Emotional contagion often focuses on passing stress, but joy can be shared, too.
Posted November 25, 2024 Reviewed by Davia Sills
Key points
- Emotions can be contagious, and that includes the positive as well as the negative.
- Therapy dogs can be a source of joy contagion in classrooms for both students and teachers.
- Educators are encouraged to reflect on how joy contagion might be introduced in their classrooms.
Emotional contagion has been described as “The tendency to mimic and synchronize facial expressions, vocalizations, postures, and movements with those of another person’s and, consequently, to converge emotionally” (Hatfield, 2009, p. 492).
Increasingly researchers are investigating the notion of emotional contagion—the transferring of emotions from one individual to another in a shared context. Admittedly, the bulk of research to date skews negative and sees researchers keen to understand how stress is passed from a high-stress individual to others.
An illustration of this is found in research by Oberle and Schonert-Reichl who sought to investigate emotional contagion in classrooms and how stress might be shared or spill over in school. Cortisol was measured in 400 students in 4th through 7th grade, along with their 16 teachers, and findings revealed that higher morning cortisol levels in students could be significantly predicted from higher burnout levels in classroom teachers. As argued by these researchers, “Whereas positive and supportive environments can help students thrive, stressful classroom environments can jeopardize healthy development and success in school” (p. 30).
Emotional contagion has even been studied between dog handlers and their therapy dog. In an effort to explore whether stress from handlers might be passed to therapy dogs, Silas and colleagues examined the self-reports of stress from 40 handlers and behavioral observations of their therapy dogs. Their findings revealed that in handlers with elevated pre-session stress, dogs showed higher signs of stress than did their counterparts characterized by moderate or low stress.
To a lesser extent, researchers have investigated positive emotional contagion and even whether positive emotions might be transferred via social media. In a study of over 689,000 Facebook users, researchers surmised that: "When positive expressions were reduced, people produced fewer positive posts and more negative posts; when negative expressions were reduced, the opposite pattern occurred. These results indicate that emotions expressed by others on Facebook influence our own emotions, constituting experimental evidence for massive-scale contagion via social networks" (Kramer et al., 2014, p. 8788). Readers are also directed to Robert Puff's Psychology Today blog entry titled "Is Happiness Contagious?"), where Puff highlights research comparing happiness versus sadness contagion.
In an attempt to activate joy contagion in a public school classroom, Henry, a therapy dog from the University of British Columbia’s B.A.R.K. program, was introduced to a 2nd and 3rd grade class. Students learned of the dog’s history (a rescue shelter mutt with three legs) and hypothesized as to how he might have lost a limb. Next, students were invited to meet and pet Henry if they so desired.
Students practiced asking the handler for permission to pet Henry and learned how to ask the dog for consent with a lowered open palm. Students were then invited to complete an assignment in response to the prompt: What makes Henry special, and what makes you special? Surprisingly, few students drew or wrote about Henry missing a leg and rather focused on his size or his fur and markings. Students celebrated their own uniqueness in their drawings of themselves, highlighting their varied qualities and talents.
As research emerges around emotional contagion in classrooms, educators are encouraged to purposefully introduce joy contagion in their classrooms as a means of enriching the climate of the classroom. This can also help reduce factors contributing to professional burnout and serve as a way of enhancing curriculum for students.
References
Hatfield, E., Rapson, R. L., & Le, Y.-C. L. (2009). Emotional contagion and empathy. In J. Decety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 19–30). Boston Review. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262012973.003.0003
Kramer, A. D. I., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, 111 (24), 8788-8790. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320040111
Oberle, E., & Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (2016). Stress contagion in the classroom? The link between classroom teacher burnout and morning cortisol in elementary school students Social Science Medicine, 159, 30-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.04.031
Silas, H. J., Binfet, J. T., & Ford, A. (2019). Therapeutic for all? Observational assessments of therapy canine stress in an on-campus stress reduction program. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 32, 6–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2019.03.009