Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Oxytocin

Dogs, Humans, and the Oxytocin-Mediated Strong Social Bond

Mutual gazing by dogs but not wolves increases oxytocin levels in humans.

For many people dogs are considered to be family members and their best friends. Indeed, because of the long period of close association between dogs and humans, a deep bond has evolved (for numerous references and discussion please click here).

While the existence of this co-evolved and special bond is a given, researchers have also been interested in what might mediate this close connection and if a similar relationship exists between wolves, from whom dogs have emerged, and humans. And now, some important light has been shed on these questions. A paper published this week by Miho Nagasawa and his colleagues titled "Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds" in the prestigious journal Science shows that indeed, there is something unique about human-dog bonds. The summary and conclusions of their essay, available now online, read as follows.

Human-like modes of communication, including mutual gaze, in dogs may have been acquired during domestication with humans. We show that gazing behavior from dogs, but not wolves, increased urinary oxytocin concentrations in owners, which consequently facilitated owners’ affiliation and increased oxytocin concentration in dogs. Further, nasally administered oxytocin increased gazing behavior in dogs, which in turn increased urinary oxytocin concentrations in owners. These findings support the existence of an interspecies oxytocin-mediated positive loop facilitated and modulated by gazing, which may have supported the coevolution of human-dog bonding by engaging common modes of communicating social attachment.

The researchers conclude:

In the present study, urinary oxytocin concentrations in owners and dogs were affected by the dog’s gaze and the duration of dog-touching. In contrast, mutual gaze between hand-raised wolves and their owners was not detected, nor was there an increase of urinary oxytocin in either wolves or their owners after a 30-min experimental interaction (experiment 1). Moreover, the nasal administration of oxytocin increased the total amount of time that female dogs gazed at their owners and, in turn, urinary oxytocin concentrations in owners (experiment 2)... These results support the existence of a self-perpetuating oxytocin-mediated positive loop in human-dog relationships that is similar to that of human mother-infant relations. Human-dog interaction by dogs’ human-like gazing behavior brought on social rewarding effects due to oxytocin release in both humans and dogs and followed the deepening of mutual relationships, which led to interspecies bonding.

Others have written about this important research including Duke University's Evan MacLean and Brian Hare in an essay called "Dogs hijack the human bonding pathway." They note, "Nagasawa et al. report the strongest test yet of the idea that humans and dogs are locked in an oxytocin feedback loop that is mediated in part through mutual gaze—sustained eye contact between human and dog." To demonstrate there was a causal relationship "Nagasawa et al. administered oxytocin to a new group of dogs before they interacted with their owners. Not only did the authors see an increase in the extent of mutual gaze between owners and dogs, but they also detected an increase in oxytocin in the dog owners as a result. Oxytocin administered to dogs increased oxytocin concentration in their owners through increased mutual gaze—however, this effect occurred only with female dogs." (For further discussion of this research please see Jan Hoffman's "The Look of Love Is in the Dog’s Eyes.")

Beware the Big "O"

This landmark study by Dr. Miho Nagasawa and his colleagues helps to explain how dogs became an intimate part of our own history. And, while oxytocin is indeed important for human-dog bonding and for dogs paying close attention to us (see "The Bond Between Dogs And Humans May Boil Down To One Amazing Molecule"), it's important to remember that, according to experts such as Paul Zak, a professor of neurology at Loma Linda University in California, oxytocin "is one of the many signals in the brain that motivates social behaviors... [and] The brain is much more complicated than one chemical." Previously I've also cautioned about over-playing the singular role of what I call the "big O"—oxytocin—and noted perhaps we will soon see "Big O" pills or sprays on the market. While there are practical difficulties in getting oxytocin into the brain, if it's profitable drug companies will find a way.

Whether or not oxytocin turns out to be the only reason behind the strong, enduring, and reciprocal human-dog social bond, this research is incredibly important for learning more about dogs and us and lays the groundwork for future work in this fascinating area of anthrozoology. Indeed, some researchers question the uniqueness of the human-dog bond. I'm sure there will be much more to report in the near future. So, please stand by.

Marc Bekoff's latest books are Jasper's story: Saving moon bears (with Jill Robinson), Ignoring nature no more: The case for compassionate conservation, Why dogs hump and bees get depressed, and Rewilding our hearts: Building pathways of compassion and coexistence. The Jane effect: Celebrating Jane Goodall (edited with Dale Peterson) has recently been published. (marcbekoff.com; @MarcBekoff)

advertisement
More from Marc Bekoff Ph.D.
More from Psychology Today