Skip to main content

Verified by Psychology Today

Education

Preschool Isn't Prep—It's Prime Time for Brain Growth

Why critical thinking begins in the crib.

Key points

  • Early childhood is when thinking, not just learning, begins.
  • Preschool is the start of school, not prep for it.
  • Brains are built not through passive instruction but through experience.

The term "preschool" implies that formal learning starts later as if the early years are merely preparation for what comes next. This perspective is profoundly misleading. The years before age five aren’t just a warm-up; they provide a crucial window when the brain is most adept at learning—not just memorizing facts but also developing essential thinking skills. The science is clear: young children's brains are biologically ready for exploration, analysis, and problem-solving. Acknowledging this doesn't mean imposing academic drills on toddlers; instead, it involves creating environments that foster their innate curiosity to understand the world.

The Brain's Critical Learning Window

From birth to around age five, a child's brain forms over a million new neural connections every second (Center on the Developing Child, 2023). These connections are influenced by experiences, particularly social interactions, play, and exposure to new stimuli. This period is termed a "critical phase" of brain plasticity, during which learning occurs more rapidly and profoundly than at any other time. Synaptic density reaches its peak in these early years, making young children particularly skilled at developing foundational cognitive and emotional abilities.

Importantly, this isn’t solely about memorizing colors or learning to count. The developing brain is designed to construct the framework for thinking: categorizing, comparing, predicting outcomes, and understanding how to learn. Children don't merely receive information—they actively develop theories about the world and test them through play and observation. Alison Gopnik, a developmental psychologist, argues that young children resemble scientists more than sponges, constantly experimenting and drawing conclusions from their surroundings (Gopnik et al., 2009).

Critical Thinking Begins in the Crib

The foundation for critical thinking—questioning, reasoning, evaluating—is established much earlier than most people think. Even infants exhibit signs of basic reasoning. For instance, studies have shown that babies look longer at outcomes that defy logical expectations, such as disappearing or floating objects (Baillargeon, 1987). This indicates that they are not passive observers; they notice inconsistencies and are surprised by violations of natural laws. Toddlers and preschoolers build on this by asking questions, often exhaustively, to test their understanding. "Why is the sky blue?" is not merely an expression of curiosity; it shows the development of a model of cause and effect.

When we talk about "critical thinking" in older students, we usually mean the ability to analyze ideas, make judgments, and solve problems. Open-ended play, storytelling, collaborative activities, and exploratory dialogue lay the groundwork for this ability in early childhood. What's often missing in early childhood settings isn't the capacity of the children—it's the recognition from adults that thinking, not just behavior, matters most.

Rethinking "Preschool" as the Start of School

The term "preschool" implies that real learning begins in kindergarten or first grade. This outdated view affects how we design early learning programs. Too often, they are seen as babysitting with a bit of enrichment. But this misses the point. If the brain is most plastic and capable before age five, we should treat early education as the centerpiece of learning—not the preamble.

A major reform is needed: early learning environments should be redefined as the first stage of formal education, not its precursor. That means shifting funding, training, and curriculum to reflect the importance of this stage. The U.S. lags behind many countries in recognizing early childhood education as a public good. Instead of seeing preschool as optional or secondary, it should be seen as a foundational investment in cognitive development and long-term academic outcomes.

A Program for Reform

To align with the science, we need a systemic shift. A reformed early education program should include:

  • Universal Access: Make high-quality early education available to all families, regardless of income. This will reduce inequities and maximize every child's developmental potential.
  • Play-Based, Inquiry-Driven Learning: Avoid worksheets and rote memorization. Classrooms should be structured around discovery, dialogue, and open-ended problem-solving.
  • Educator Training: Early educators should be trained in classroom management and child development, cognitive science, and how to facilitate critical thinking in young learners.
  • Integration with K–12: Early education should be integrated into the broader educational system, with clear learning goals that evolve naturally into the elementary curriculum.
  • Parent Involvement: Equip parents with tools and knowledge about early brain development to support learning at home in ways that reinforce school experiences.

This model is not about pushing academics early but about respecting children's thinking capacity. Science doesn't say children need to read by age three; it says they need rich, responsive environments that challenge their minds and nurture their questions.

The Bottom Line

The notion that preschool merely serves as preparation for real school overlooks what neuroscience has established: the early years are when the brain is most receptive to learning, not just in terms of language or memory, but in critical thinking itself. If we consider preschool optional or less important, we waste a unique opportunity to cultivate the mental habits that foster lifelong learning. Education doesn't begin in kindergarten—it starts at birth. It's time our educational systems caught up.

References

Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½- and 4½-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 655–664. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.655

Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University. (2023). Brain architecture. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/science/key-concepts/brain-architecture/

Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (2009). The scientist in the crib: What early learning tells us about the mind. HarperPaperbacks.

advertisement
More from Sam Goldstein Ph.D.
More from Psychology Today