Media
Rings of Power and Privilege
Popular media promotes negative biases towards people with facial differences.
Posted November 14, 2022 Reviewed by Abigail Fagan
Key points
- Most people have a negative implicit bias against people with facial anomalies, such as scars, birthmarks, and developmental abnormalities.
- Research shows that culture plays an important role in engendering these biases.
- Popular movies traffic in these stereotypes when villainy is marked by facial differences.
- We need to counter stigma and discrimination experienced daily by people with facial anomalies.

I recently finished watching Rings of Power, the latest video streaming installment of JRR Tolkien’s epic fantasy. The book series on which this show is based had a strong hold on my high school and college cohort in the mid 1970s, perhaps in the same way that JK Rawlings’ fantasies gripped adolescents coming of age earlier this century. Tolkien served up an adventure saga on a grand scale in which many kids immersed themselves. The “fellowship” suggested that a motley crew of humans, hobbits, elves, and dwarves could band together and fight evil. Diversity and inclusion were on display as a force for good. (In a world of monarchs and bloodlines, equity was never a consideration). Even as I reveled in the recent lavish production, with a first season budget of around half a billion dollars, I felt a disquiet. A disquiet to which I was insensitive half a century ago.
Psychological and neural responses to facial anomalies
For better and worse, our looks matter. Psychologists have long identified a beauty-is-good stereotype. That is, attractive people receive all sorts of unearned privileges in life. They are hired more easily, given higher pay, and receive lesser punishments for infractions than their less attractive counterparts. We found an analogous facial anomaly-is-bad bias, in which people who have scars, birthmarks, and developmental differences are viewed as being less intelligent, hard-working, trustworthy, and competent (Jamrozik et al., 2017).
Many people harbor such biases implicitly; our brains track them even if we don't (Hartung et al., 2019). Based on our research, three brain structures are critical for these effects (Workman et al., 2021). They include areas in our visual cortex, called the fusiform gyri, and in the left amygdala. Parts of the fusiform gyrus are tuned to processing faces, and the amygdala is implicated in processing various emotional states, especially fear and anxiety, tied to their social relevance. Neural activity in these areas correlated with the degree of implicit bias held by people enrolled in the study. The left amygdala activity was further modulated by individual differences in people’s dispositions. Those who reported that they feel less empathy towards others, and also believe that the world is generally just (i.e., people get what they deserve), had a stronger neural response in their amygdala to faces with such differences.
A standard interpretation of such a seemingly "wired” in the brain response is that we evolved a mechanism of pathogen sensitivity. Pathogens, whether in plants, animals, or humans, can cause morphological disfigurements. Since we wish to avoid pathogens, humans developed unconscious reflexive responses designed to avoid potential carriers of pathogens, or people who are signaling compromised health that makes them susceptible to infections.
Is this pathogen sensitivity account the whole story?
Cultural contributions
Pathogen sensitivity presumably was coded in our brains during the Pleistocene era when we lived in small groups as hunter and gatherer nomads. If this avoidance mechanism were necessary to preserve health in small populations, we would expect a similar bias to be conserved in modern day hunter gatherers. To find out if such biases exist, we conducted a study with the Hadza people of Tanzania (Workman et al., 2022). The Hadza are a hunter gatherer group who live in small nomadic groups around Lake Eyasi in the central Rift Valley. Using digital methods, we created face images based on Hadza physiognomy and rendered scars on some of them. Conducting research in the field with the Hadza is not exactly similar to enrolling college undergraduate students in a computer-based experiment for class credit. The Hadza in our study were shown pictures and asked if they thought the person was a good hunter/gatherer and if they thought the person had a good heart. These questions targeted judgments of competence and warmth--two factors important to how we form stereotypes.
Looking at the data, it first appeared that the Hadza also formed stereotypes of people with scars. On closer examination, the pattern of their responses were far more interesting. Some Hadza have minimal contact with the outside world. The degree of contact can be assessed by asking questions like, is the name Nelson Mandela familiar, or, do you know words in Swahili? When we factored exposure to non-Hadza culture into the analysis of the data, all hints of a stereotype vanished. As best we can tell, biases against faces with anomalies were absent in this hunter gatherer group, and only start to emerge with outside cultural contact. We are teaching our brains the anomalous-is-bad bias.
Popular media
In the video series Rings of Power the elves are contrasted with the orcs. The elves are good, immortal, golden, and beautiful. The orcs are evil, undead, dark, and distorted. Half a billion dollars were spent on this visual imagery depicting good and evil. Our popular media is replete with similar tropes. Just about every villain in the Bond, Marvel, and Star Wars universes is depicted with facial anomalies. Our children are fed the same visual diet of good and evil in movies like Beauty and the Beast and The Lion King. One need not bother with developing much of a back story for why a character becomes bad. Their face tells the story.
Organizations like Face Equality International, About Face, and Changing Faces are trying to counter the deluge of these messages. They are trying to eliminate the stigma and discrimination that people with facial differences encounter on a daily basis and promote equity for their constituency through advocacy and education. Theirs are relatively soft voices rallying against a loud Hollywood roar. Those in media rings of power and privilege could better hear members of Changing Face’s simple but trenchant message: “I am more than just your villain.”
References
Hartung, F., Jamrozik, A., Rosen, M. E., Aguirre, G., Sarwer, D. B., & Chatterjee, A. (2019). Behavioural and Neural Responses to Facial Disfigurement. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 8021. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44408-8
Jamrozik, A., Oraa Ali, M., Sarwer, D. B., & Chatterjee, A. (2017). More Than Skin Deep: Judgments of Individuals With Facial Disfigurement. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13(1), 117-129. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000147
Workman, C. I., Humphries, S., Hartung, F., Aguirre, G. K., Kable, J. W., & Chatterjee, A. (2021). Morality is in the eye of the beholder: the neurocognitive basis of the “anomalous-is-bad” stereotype. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1494(1), 3-17. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14575
Workman, C. I., Smith, K. M., Apicella, C. L., & Chatterjee, A. (2022). Evidence against the “anomalous-is-bad” stereotype in Hadza hunter gatherers. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 8693. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12440-w