Looking in the Cultural Mirror

How understanding race and culture helps us answer the question: "Who am I?"

The Trouble With Double-Blind Placebo Studies

Two flaws in double-blind placebo studies

Prescription placebos used in research and practice
Double-blind placebo studies have been called the gold standard for testing medications, especially psychiatric ones. Since the placebo effect is a psychological effect, it stands to reason that its effect on psychological conditions should be an especially potent one.

Double-blind placebo studies improve on experiments that compare the response of people taking a pill (or other treatment) to those who do not. The problem with these experiments is that they mix up the placebo effect (taking a pill) with the treatment effect (the medication in the pill). Placebo studies separate these effects. In addition, in double blind studies neither the people involved in giving the pill nor the ones taking it know if it is a placebo. That way, the danger of experimenters nonverbally communicating their expectation that the pill will work (or not) is overcome.

Find a Therapist

Search for a mental health professional near you.

Typically, there are three groups: a control group, a placebo group, and a treatment group. The control group doesn't get a pill; if they get better over time--as is often the case with depressed people, for example--their improvement sets a baseline against which to compare the other two groups. When people in the placebo group improve more than the controls, their increased improvement can be attributed to the belief that the pill works. Only when people in the treatment group improve more than those in the placebo group can that additional improvement be attributed to the medication in the pill.

Unfortunately, there are significant problems with double-blind placebo studies. These have long been known in the research world, but for some reason word hasn't gotten out to the public. Here are two important ones.

Problem #1--In real life, when patients receive a prescription, they are implicitly told "This is real medicine." In double-blind placebo studies, subjects are explicitly told "This pill might be real medicine, or it might be a placebo." Obviously, these lead to different sets of expectations, with different effects.

The solution to this shortcoming is the balanced placebo design. In addition to a control group, it has two placebo groups and two treatment groups. People in both placebo groups receive a placebo, but one group is told that it is a placebo, and the other group is told that it is real medicine. And people in both treatment groups receive the medicine, but one group is told that it is a placebo, and the other group is told that it is real medicine.

The difficulty with the balanced placebo design is an ethical one--it involves deceiving participants and violating the principle of informed consent. The fact that such studies cannot be done ethically, however, leaves the problem of effectively controlling for expectancies unresolved.

Problem #2--Active placebos. Double-blind studies respond to the objection of experimenters unintentionally communicating whether or not a pill is a placebo. But they don't respond to the objection of the pill communicating that information. While the placebo pill and real pill look identical, the placebo pill is inert, but the real pill has real biological effects. Therefore, even though the real pill may not improve the condition it is supposed to treat, people taking it may be able to feel those effects. This could lead them to conclude "I must be in the treatment group." This is a more positive expectancy than "I don't know if I'm in the treatment group or the placebo group." As a result, placebo effects could be misinterpreted as treatment effects.

A partial solution would be to use active placebos instead of inert placebos. For example, nicotinic acid (niacin) causes skin flushing and itching. However, since the real pill and the active placebo pill would have different bodily effects, there is still the possibility that this difference rather than the medication in the real pill is responsible for any greater improvement in the treatment group.


Maybe instead of calling double-blind placebo studies the gold standard, we should call them the brass standard--far from perfect, but unfortunately the best practical solution available.


Image Source:
Prescription placebos used in research and practice
Author NIH, objects donated by Elaine and Arthur Shapiro
http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Four_Placebos.jpg

 

Check out my most recent book, The Myth of Race, which debunks common misconceptions, as well as my other books at http://amazon.com/Jefferson-M.-Fish/e/B001H6NFUI

The Myth of Race is available on Amazon http://amzn.to/10ykaRU and Barnes & Noble http://bit.ly/XPbB6E

Friend/Like me on Facebook: http://www.facebook.com/JeffersonFishAuthor

Follow me on Twitter: www.twitter.com/@jeffersonfish

Visit my website: www.jeffersonfish.com

Jefferson M. Fish, Ph.D., a Professor Emeritus of Psychology at St. John's University, has authored and edited 12 books, including The Myth of Race.

more...

Subscribe to Looking in the Cultural Mirror

Current Issue

Love & Lust

Who says marriage is where desire goes to die?